Rational singularity: Difference between revisions

From formulasearchengine
Jump to navigation Jump to search
en>AnomieBOT
m Dating maintenance tags: {{Citation needed}}
en>Trappist the monk
m →‎References: replace mr template with mr parameter in CS1 templates; using AWB
 
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Cleanup|date=July 2009}}
Ed is what people contact me and my spouse doesn't like it at all. Some time ago he selected to reside in North Carolina and he doesn't strategy on altering it. My working day occupation is a travel agent. To climb is something I truly appreciate doing.<br><br>Here is my web site - free psychic reading - [http://m-card.co.kr/xe/mcard_2013_promote01/29877 http://m-card.co.kr] -
 
[[Image:EyeOpticsV400y.jpg|thumb|400px|Diagram showing visual angle V.]]
 
The '''visual angle''' is the angle a viewed object subtends at the eye, usually stated in [[degrees of arc]].
It also is called the object's [[angular size]].
 
The diagram on the right shows an observer's eye  looking at a frontal extent (the vertical arrow) that has a  linear size <math>S</math>, located in the distance <math>D</math> from point <math>O</math>.
 
For present purposes, point <math>O</math> can represent  the eye's [[nodal point]]s at about the center of the lens, and also represent the center of the eye's [[entrance pupil]] that is only a few millimeters in front of the lens.
 
The three lines from object endpoint <math>A</math> heading toward the eye indicate the bundle of  light rays that pass through the cornea, pupil and lens to form an optical image of  endpoint <math>A</math> on the [[retina]] at point <math>a</math>.
The central line of the bundle represents the [[chief ray]]
 
Likewise for object point <math>B</math> and its retinal image at <math>b</math>.
 
The  visual angle <math>V</math> is the angle between the chief rays for <math>A</math> and <math>B</math>.
 
==Measuring and computing==
The visual angle <math>V</math> can be measured directly using a [[theodolite]] placed at point <math>O</math>.
 
Or, it can be calculated,<ref>{{Cite web| url = http://www.yorku.ca/eye/visangle.htm | title = Calculation of Visual Angle | work = The Joy of Visual Perception: A Web Book  | first = Peter K. | last = Kaiser | publisher = [[York University]]}}</ref> in radians, using the formula, <math>V = 2 \arctan\left(\frac{S}{2D}\right)</math>.
 
However, for visual angles smaller than about 10 degrees, this simpler formula provides very close approximations:
 
: <math>\tan V = \frac{S}{D}.</math>
 
==The retinal image and visual angle==
As the above sketch shows, a [[real image]] of the object is formed on the retina between points <math>a</math> and <math>b</math>. (See [[visual system]]). For small angles, the size of this retinal image <math>R</math> is
 
:<math>\frac{R}{n} = \tan V,</math>
 
where <math>n</math> is the distance from the nodal points to the retina, about 17&nbsp;mm.
 
==Examples==
If one looks at a one-centimeter object at a distance of one meter and a two-centimeter object at a distance of two meters, both subtend the same visual angle of about 0.01 rad or 0.57°.  Thus they have the same retinal image size <math>R \approx 0.17 \text{ mm}</math>.
 
That is just a bit larger than the retinal image size for the moon, which is about <math>0.15 \text{ mm}</math>, because, with <math>S = 2160 \text{ miles}</math>, and <math>D</math> averaging <math>238,000 \text{ miles}</math>, <math>V \approx 0.009 \text{ rad}</math><math>\approx 0.52 \text{ deg}</math>.
 
Also, for some  easy observations, if one holds one's index finger at arm's length, the width of the index fingernail subtends approximately one degree, and the width of the thumb at the first joint subtends approximately two degrees.<ref>{{Cite journal| last = O'Shea | first = Robert P. | year = 1991 | title = Thumb's rule tested: Visual angle of thumb's width is about 2 deg | journal = [[Perception (journal)|Perception]] | volume = 20 | issue = 3 | pages = 415–418 | doi = 10.1068/p200415 | url = http://www.perceptionweb.com/abstract.cgi?id=p200415 | pmid=1762884}}</ref>
 
Therefore, if one is interested in the performance of the eye or the first processing steps in the [[visual cortex]], it does not make sense to refer to the absolute size of a viewed object (its linear size <math>S</math>). What matters is the visual angle <math>V</math> which determines the size of the retinal image.
 
==Terminological confusions==
In astronomy the term ''apparent size'' refers to the physical angle <math>V</math> or [[angular diameter]].
 
But in [[psychophysics]] and [[experimental psychology]] the adjective "apparent" refers to a person's subjective experience.
So, "apparent size" has  referred to how large an object looks, also often called its "perceived size".
 
Additional confusion has occurred because there are two qualitatively different "size" experiences for a viewed object.<ref>Joynson, 1949, McCready, 1965, 1985, Baird, 1970</ref> One is the perceived  visual angle <math>V'</math> (or apparent visual angle) which is the subjective correlate of <math>V</math>, also called the object's perceived or apparent angular size.
The perceived  visual angle is best defined as the difference between the perceived directions of the object's endpoints from oneself.<ref>Joynson, 1949, McCready, 1965, 1985</ref>
 
The other "size" experience  is the object's perceived linear size <math>S'</math> (or apparent linear size) which is the subjective correlate of <math>S</math>, the object's physical width or height or diameter.
 
Widespread  use of the ambiguous  terms "apparent size" and "perceived size" without specifying the units of  measure has caused confusion.
 
==Visual angle and the visual cortex==
The brain's [[primary visual cortex]] (area V1 or Brodmann area 17) contains a spatially [[isomorphic]] representation of the retina (see [[retinotopy]]).  Loosely speaking,  it is a distorted "map" of the retina.
Accordingly, the size <math>R</math> of a given retinal image determines the extent of the neural activity pattern eventually generated in area V1 by the associated retinal activity pattern.
 
Murray, Boyaci, & Kersten (2006) recently used [[Functional magnetic resonance imaging]]  [[fMRI]] to show, convincingly,{{peacock inline|date=April 2012}} that an increase in a viewed target's visual angle,  which increases <math>R</math>, increases the extent of the corresponding neural activity pattern in area V1.
 
Their most important finding, however, relates to the perceived visual angle <math>V'</math> and to the visual angle illusion.
 
==Angular size illusion and area V1==
The Murray, et al. observers viewed a flat picture with two discs that subtended the same visual angle <math>V</math> and formed retinal images of the same size <math>R</math>, but the perceived angular size <math>V'</math> of one was about 17% larger than for the other, due to differences in the background patterns for the disks.
 
The major discovery was that the sizes of the area V1 activity patterns related to the disks were unequal, despite the fact that the retinal images were the same size.  This size difference in area V1 correlated almost perfectly with the 17% illusory difference between the perceived visual angles.
 
These new findings are crucial for theories of visual space perception and especially any [[visual angle illusion]].<ref>{{Cite web| title = An Analysis of the Experiment by Murray, S. O., Boyaci, H., & Kersten, D. (2006). "The representation of perceived angular size in human primary visual cortex." | url = http://facstaff.uww.edu/mccreadd/appendixBB%20htm.htm | date = 5 February 2007 | work = The Moon Illusion Explained | first = Don | last = McCready}}</ref>
 
==See also==
*[[Visual acuity]]
*[[Visual angle illusion]]
 
==Notes==
{{Reflist}}
 
==References==
* Baird, J. C. (1970).  ''Psychophysical analysis of visual space.'' Oxford, London: Pergamon Press.
* Joynson, R. B. (1949). The problem of size and distance. ''Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1'', 119–135.
* McCready, D. (1965). Size-distance perception and accommodation-convergence micropsia: A critique. ''Vision Research. 5,'' 189–206.
* McCready, D. (1985). On size, distance and visual angle perception. ''Perception & Psychophysics, 37'', 323–334.
* Murray, S.O., Boyaci, H, & Kersten, D. (2006) [http://www.ski.org/Visproc/pdf/Murray-Boyaci-Kersten-2006.pdf The representation of perceived angular size in human primary visual cortex.] ''Nature Neuroscience, 9,'' 429–434 (1 March 2006).
* McCready, D. [http://facstaff.uww.edu/mccreadd/ The Moon Illusion Explained].
 
==External links==
*[http://www.smbs.buffalo.edu/oph/ped/IVAC/IVAC.html The University at Buffalo's Interactive Visual Acuity Chart] for the display of letters or symbols for a specified Snellen line on your computer monitor at exactly the right size (note: you must follow the instructions for calibration).
 
{{Use dmy dates|date=September 2010}}
 
{{DEFAULTSORT:Visual Angle}}
[[Category:Vision]]
[[Category:Angle]]

Latest revision as of 23:11, 25 September 2014

Ed is what people contact me and my spouse doesn't like it at all. Some time ago he selected to reside in North Carolina and he doesn't strategy on altering it. My working day occupation is a travel agent. To climb is something I truly appreciate doing.

Here is my web site - free psychic reading - http://m-card.co.kr -